Data, measurement and empirical methods in the science of science
The advent of large-scale datasets that trace the workings of science has encouraged researchers from many different disciplinary backgrounds to turn scientific methods into science itself, cultivating a rapidly expanding ‘science of science’. This Review considers this growing, multidisciplinary literature through the lens of data, measurement and empirical methods. We discuss the purposes, strengths and limitations of major empirical approaches, seeking to increase understanding of the field’s diverse methodologies and expand researchers’ toolkits. Overall, new empirical developments provide enormous capacity to test traditional beliefs and conceptual frameworks about science, discover factors associated with scientific productivity, predict scientific outcomes and design policies that facilitate scientific progress.
Similar content being viewed by others
SciSciNet: A large-scale open data lake for the science of science research
Article Open access 01 June 2023
A dataset for measuring the impact of research data and their curation
Article Open access 03 May 2024
Envisioning a “science diplomacy 2.0”: on data, global challenges, and multi-layered networks
Article Open access 06 November 2020
Main
Scientific advances are a key input to rising standards of living, health and the capacity of society to confront grand challenges, from climate change to the COVID-19 pandemic 1,2,3 . A deeper understanding of how science works and where innovation occurs can help us to more effectively design science policy and science institutions, better inform scientists’ own research choices, and create and capture enormous value for science and humanity. Building on these key premises, recent years have witnessed substantial development in the ‘science of science’ 4,5,6,7,8,9 , which uses large-scale datasets and diverse computational toolkits to unearth fundamental patterns behind scientific production and use.
The idea of turning scientific methods into science itself is long-standing. Since the mid-20th century, researchers from different disciplines have asked central questions about the nature of scientific progress and the practice, organization and impact of scientific research. Building on these rich historical roots, the field of the science of science draws upon many disciplines, ranging from information science to the social, physical and biological sciences to computer science, engineering and design. The science of science closely relates to several strands and communities of research, including metascience, scientometrics, the economics of science, research on research, science and technology studies, the sociology of science, metaknowledge and quantitative science studies 5 . There are noticeable differences between some of these communities, mostly around their historical origins and the initial disciplinary composition of researchers forming these communities. For example, metascience has its origins in the clinical sciences and psychology, and focuses on rigour, transparency, reproducibility and other open science-related practices and topics. The scientometrics community, born in library and information sciences, places a particular emphasis on developing robust and responsible measures and indicators for science. Science and technology studies engage the history of science and technology, the philosophy of science, and the interplay between science, technology and society. The science of science, which has its origins in physics, computer science and sociology, takes a data-driven approach and emphasizes questions on how science works. Each of these communities has made fundamental contributions to understanding science. While they differ in their origins, these differences pale in comparison to the overarching, common interest in understanding the practice of science and its societal impact.
Three major developments have encouraged rapid advances in the science of science. The first is in data 9 : modern databases include millions of research articles, grant proposals, patents and more. This windfall of data traces scientific activity in remarkable detail and at scale. The second development is in measurement: scholars have used data to develop many new measures of scientific activities and examine theories that have long been viewed as important but difficult to quantify. The third development is in empirical methods: thanks to parallel advances in data science, network science, artificial intelligence and econometrics, researchers can study relationships, make predictions and assess science policy in powerful new ways. Together, new data, measurements and methods have revealed fundamental new insights about the inner workings of science and scientific progress itself.
With multiple approaches, however, comes a key challenge. As researchers adhere to norms respected within their disciplines, their methods vary, with results often published in venues with non-overlapping readership, fragmenting research along disciplinary boundaries. This fragmentation challenges researchers’ ability to appreciate and understand the value of work outside of their own discipline, much less to build directly on it for further investigations.
Recognizing these challenges and the rapidly developing nature of the field, this paper reviews the empirical approaches that are prevalent in this literature. We aim to provide readers with an up-to-date understanding of the available datasets, measurement constructs and empirical methodologies, as well as the value and limitations of each. Owing to space constraints, this Review does not cover the full technical details of each method, referring readers to related guides to learn more. Instead, we will emphasize why a researcher might favour one method over another, depending on the research question.
Beyond a positive understanding of science, a key goal of the science of science is to inform science policy. While this Review mainly focuses on empirical approaches, with its core audience being researchers in the field, the studies reviewed are also germane to key policy questions. For example, what is the appropriate scale of scientific investment, in what directions and through what institutions 10,11 ? Are public investments in science aligned with public interests 12 ? What conditions produce novel or high-impact science 13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20 ? How do the reward systems of science influence the rate and direction of progress 13,21,22,23,24 , and what governs scientific reproducibility 25,26,27 ? How do contributions evolve over a scientific career 28,29,30,31,32 , and how may diversity among scientists advance scientific progress 33,34,35 , among other questions relevant to science policy 36,37 .
Overall, this review aims to facilitate entry to science of science research, expand researcher toolkits and illustrate how diverse research approaches contribute to our collective understanding of science. Section 2 reviews datasets and data linkages. Section 3 reviews major measurement constructs in the science of science. Section 4 considers a range of empirical methods, focusing on one study to illustrate each method and briefly summarizing related examples and applications. Section 5 concludes with an outlook for the science of science.
Data
Historically, data on scientific activities were difficult to collect and were available in limited quantities. Gathering data could involve manually tallying statistics from publications 38,39 , interviewing scientists 16,40 , or assembling historical anecdotes and biographies 13,41 . Analyses were typically limited to a specific domain or group of scientists. Today, massive datasets on scientific production and use are at researchers’ fingertips 42,43,44 . Armed with big data and advanced algorithms, researchers can now probe questions previously not amenable to quantification and with enormous increases in scope and scale, as detailed below.
Publication datasets cover papers from nearly all scientific disciplines, enabling analyses of both general and domain-specific patterns. Commonly used datasets include the Web of Science (WoS), PubMed, CrossRef, ORCID, OpenCitations, Dimensions and OpenAlex. Datasets incorporating papers’ text (CORE) 45,46,47 , data entities (DataCite) 48,49 and peer review reports (Publons) 33,50,51 have also become available. These datasets further enable novel measurement, for example, representations of a paper’s content 52,53 , novelty 15,54 and interdisciplinarity 55 .
Notably, databases today capture more diverse aspects of science beyond publications, offering a richer and more encompassing view of research contexts and of researchers themselves (Fig. 1). For example, some datasets trace research funding to the specific publications these investments support 56,57 , allowing high-scale studies of the impact of funding on productivity and the return on public investment. Datasets incorporating job placements 58,59 , curriculum vitae 21,59 and scientific prizes 23 offer rich quantitative evidence on the social structure of science. Combining publication profiles with mentorship genealogies 60,61 , dissertations 34 and course syllabi 62,63 provides insights on mentoring and cultivating talent.
Finally, today’s scope of data extends beyond science to broader aspects of society. Altmetrics 64 captures news media and social media mentions of scientific articles. Other databases incorporate marketplace uses of science, including through patents 10 , pharmaceutical clinical trials and drug approvals 65,66 . Policy documents 67,68 help us to understand the role of science in the halls of government 69 and policy making 12,68 .
While datasets of the modern scientific enterprise have grown exponentially, they are not without limitations. As is often the case for data-driven research, drawing conclusions from specific data sources requires scrutiny and care. Datasets are typically based on published work, which may favour easy-to-publish topics over important ones (the streetlight effect) 70,71 . The publication of negative results is also rare (the file drawer problem) 72,73 . Meanwhile, English language publications account for over 90% of articles in major data sources, with limited coverage of non-English journals 74 . Publication datasets may also reflect biases in data collection across research institutions or demographic groups. Despite the open science movement, many datasets require paid subscriptions, which can create inequality in data access. Creating more open datasets for the science of science, such as OpenAlex, may not only improve the robustness and replicability of empirical claims but also increase entry to the field.
As today’s datasets become larger in scale and continue to integrate new dimensions, they offer opportunities to unveil the inner workings and external impacts of science in new ways. They can enable researchers to reach beyond previous limitations while conducting original studies of new and long-standing questions about the sciences.
Measurement
Here we discuss prominent measurement approaches in the science of science, including their purposes and limitations.
Citations
Modern publication databases typically include data on which articles and authors cite other papers and scientists. These citation linkages have been used to engage core conceptual ideas in scientific research. Here we consider two common measures based on citation information: citation counts and knowledge flows.
First, citation counts are commonly used indicators of impact. The term ‘indicator’ implies that it only approximates the concept of interest. A citation count is defined as how many times a document is cited by subsequent documents and can proxy for the importance of research papers 75,76 as well as patented inventions 77,78,79 . Rather than treating each citation equally, measures may further weight the importance of each citation, for example by using the citation network structure to produce centrality 80 , PageRank 81,82 or Eigenfactor indicators 83,84 .
Citation-based indicators have also faced criticism 84,85 . Citation indicators necessarily oversimplify the construct of impact, often ignoring heterogeneity in the meaning and use of a particular reference, the variations in citation practices across fields and institutional contexts, and the potential for reputation and power structures in science to influence citation behaviour 86,87 . Researchers have started to understand more nuanced citation behaviours ranging from negative citations 86 to citation context 47,88,89 . Understanding what a citation actually measures matters in interpreting and applying many research findings in the science of science. Evaluations relying on citation-based indicators rather than expert judgements raise questions regarding misuse 90,91,92 . Given the importance of developing indicators that can reliably quantify and evaluate science, the scientometrics community has been working to provide guidance for responsible citation practices and assessment 85 .
Second, scientists use citations to trace knowledge flows. Each citation in a paper is a link to specific previous work from which we can proxy how new discoveries draw upon existing ideas 76,93 and how knowledge flows between fields of science 94,95 , research institutions 96 , regions and nations 97,98,99 , and individuals 81 . Combinations of citation linkages can also approximate novelty 15 , disruptiveness 17,100 and interdisciplinarity 55,95,101,102 . A rapidly expanding body of work further examines citations to scientific articles from other domains (for example, patents, clinical drug trials and policy documents) to understand the applied value of science 10,12,65,66,103,104,105 .
Individuals
Analysing individual careers allows researchers to answer questions such as: How do we quantify individual scientific productivity? What is a typical career lifecycle? How are resources and credits allocated across individuals and careers? A scholar’s career can be examined through the papers they publish 30,31,106,107,108 , with attention to career progression and mobility, publication counts and citation impact, as well as grant funding 24,109,110 and prizes 111,112,113 ,
Studies of individual impact focus on output, typically approximated by the number of papers a researcher publishes and citation indicators. A popular measure for individual impact is the h-index 114 , which takes both volume and per-paper impact into consideration. Specifically, a scientist is assigned the largest value h such that they have h papers that were each cited at least h times. Later studies build on the idea of the h-index and propose variants to address limitations 115 , these variants ranging from emphasizing highly cited papers in a career 116 , to field differences 117 and normalizations 118 , to the relative contribution of an individual in collaborative works 119 .
To study dynamics in output over the lifecycle, individuals can be studied according to age, career age or the sequence of publications. A long-standing literature has investigated the relationship between age and the likelihood of outstanding achievement 28,106,111,120,121 . Recent studies further decouple the relationship between age, publication volume and per-paper citation, and measure the likelihood of producing highly cited papers in the sequence of works one produces 30,31 .
As simple as it sounds, representing careers using publication records is difficult. Collecting the full publication list of a researcher is the foundation to study individuals yet remains a key challenge, requiring name disambiguation techniques to match specific works to specific researchers. Although algorithms are increasingly capable at identifying millions of career profiles 122 , they vary in accuracy and robustness. ORCID can help to alleviate the problem by offering researchers the opportunity to create, maintain and update individual profiles themselves, and it goes beyond publications to collect broader outputs and activities 123 . A second challenge is survivorship bias. Empirical studies tend to focus on careers that are long enough to afford statistical analyses, which limits the applicability of the findings to scientific careers as a whole. A third challenge is the breadth of scientists’ activities, where focusing on publications ignores other important contributions such as mentorship and teaching, service (for example, refereeing papers, reviewing grant proposals and editing journals) or leadership within their organizations. Although researchers have begun exploring these dimensions by linking individual publication profiles with genealogical databases 61,124 , dissertations 34 , grants 109 , curriculum vitae 21 and acknowledgements 125 , scientific careers beyond publication records remain under-studied 126,127 . Lastly, citation-based indicators only serve as an approximation of individual performance with similar limitations as discussed above. The scientific community has called for more appropriate practices 85,128 , ranging from incorporating expert assessment of research contributions to broadening the measures of impact beyond publications.
Teams
Over many decades, science has exhibited a substantial and steady shift away from solo authorship towards coauthorship, especially among highly cited works 18,129,130 . In light of this shift, a research field, the science of team science 131,132 , has emerged to study the mechanisms that facilitate or hinder the effectiveness of teams. Team size can be proxied by the number of coauthors on a paper, which has been shown to predict distinctive types of advance: whereas larger teams tend to develop ideas, smaller teams tend to disrupt current ways of thinking 17 . Team characteristics can be inferred from coauthors’ backgrounds 133,134,135 , allowing quantification of a team’s diversity in terms of field, age, gender or ethnicity. Collaboration networks based on coauthorship 130,136,137,138,139 offer nuanced network-based indicators to understand individual and institutional collaborations.
However, there are limitations to using coauthorship alone to study teams 132 . First, coauthorship can obscure individual roles 140,141,142 , which has prompted institutional responses to help to allocate credit, including authorship order and individual contribution statements 56,143 . Second, coauthorship does not reflect the complex dynamics and interactions between team members that are often instrumental for team success 53,144 . Third, collaborative contributions can extend beyond coauthorship in publications to include members of a research laboratory 145 or co-principal investigators (co-PIs) on a grant 146 . Initiatives such as CRediT may help to address some of these issues by recording detailed roles for each contributor 147 .
Institutions
Research institutions, such as departments, universities, national laboratories and firms, encompass wider groups of researchers and their corresponding outputs. Institutional membership can be inferred from affiliations listed on publications or patents 148,149 , and the output of an institution can be aggregated over all its affiliated researchers 150 . Institutional research information systems (CRIS) contain more comprehensive research outputs and activities from employees.
Some research questions consider the institution as a whole, investigating the returns to research and development investment 104 , inequality of resource allocation 22 and the flow of scientists 21,148,149 . Other questions focus on institutional structures as sources of research productivity by looking into the role of peer effects 125,151,152,153 , how institutional policies impact research outcomes 154,155 and whether interdisciplinary efforts foster innovation 55 . Institution-oriented measurement faces similar limitations as with analyses of individuals and teams, including name disambiguation for a given institution and the limited capacity of formal publication records to characterize the full range of relevant institutional outcomes. It is also unclear how to allocate credit among multiple institutions associated with a paper. Moreover, relevant institutional employees extend beyond publishing researchers: interns, technicians and administrators all contribute to research endeavours 130 .
In sum, measurements allow researchers to quantify scientific production and use across numerous dimensions, but they also raise questions of construct validity: Does the proposed metric really reflect what we want to measure? Testing the construct’s validity is important, as is understanding a construct’s limits. Where possible, using alternative measurement approaches, or qualitative methods such as interviews and surveys, can improve measurement accuracy and the robustness of findings.
Empirical methods
In this section, we review two broad categories of empirical approaches (Table 1), each with distinctive goals: (1) to discover, estimate and predict empirical regularities; and (2) to identify causal mechanisms. For each method, we give a concrete example to help to explain how the method works, summarize related work for interested readers, and discuss contributions and limitations.
While machine learning can offer high predictive accuracy, successful applications to the science of science face challenges, particularly regarding interpretability. Researchers may value transparent and interpretable findings for how a given feature influences an outcome, rather than a black-box model. The lack of interpretability also raises concerns about bias and fairness. In predicting reproducible patterns from data, machine learning models inevitably include and reproduce biases embedded in these data, often in non-transparent ways. The fairness of machine learning 217 is heavily debated in applications ranging from the criminal justice system to hiring processes. Effective and responsible use of machine learning in the science of science therefore requires thoughtful partnership between humans and machines 53 to build a reliable system accessible to scrutiny and modification.
Causal approaches
The preceding methods can reveal core facts about the workings of science and develop predictive capacity. Yet, they fail to capture causal relationships, which are particularly useful in assessing policy interventions. For example, how can we test whether a science policy boosts or hinders the performance of individuals, teams or institutions? The overarching idea of causal approaches is to construct some counterfactual world where two groups are identical to each other except that one group experiences a treatment that the other group does not.
Towards causation
Before engaging in causal approaches, it is useful to first consider the interpretative challenges of observational data. As observational data emerge from mechanisms that are not fully known or measured, an observed correlation may be driven by underlying forces that were not accounted for in the analysis. This challenge makes causal inference fundamentally difficult in observational data. An awareness of this issue is the first step in confronting it. It further motivates intermediate empirical approaches, including the use of matching strategies and fixed effects, that can help to confront (although not fully eliminate) the inference challenge. We first consider these approaches before turning to more fully causal methods.
Matching. Matching utilizes rich information to construct a control group that is similar to the treatment group on as many observable characteristics as possible before the treatment group is exposed to the treatment. Inferences can then be made by comparing the treatment and the matched control groups. Exact matching applies to categorical values, such as country, gender, discipline or affiliation 35,218 . Coarsened exact matching considers percentile bins of continuous variables and matches observations in the same bin 133 . Propensity score matching estimates the probability of receiving the ‘treatment’ on the basis of the controlled variables and uses the estimates to match treatment and control groups, which reduces the matching task from comparing the values of multiple covariates to comparing a single value 24,219 . Dynamic matching is useful for longitudinally matching variables that change over time 220,221 .
Fixed effects. Fixed effects are a powerful and now standard tool in controlling for confounders. A key requirement for using fixed effects is that there are multiple observations on the same subject or entity (person, field, institution and so on) 222,223,224 . The fixed effect works as a dummy variable that accounts for the role of any fixed characteristic of that entity. Consider the finding where gender-diverse teams produce higher-impact papers than same-gender teams do 225 . A confounder may be that individuals who tend to write high-impact papers may also be more likely to work in gender-diverse teams. By including individual fixed effects, one accounts for any fixed characteristics of individuals (such as IQ, cultural background or previous education) that might drive the relationship of interest.
In sum, matching and fixed effects methods reduce potential sources of bias in interpreting relationships between variables. Yet, confounders may persist in these studies. For instance, fixed effects do not control for unobserved factors that change with time within the given entity (for example, access to funding or new skills). Identifying casual effects convincingly will then typically require distinct research methods that we turn to next.
Quasi-experiments
Researchers in economics and other fields have developed a range of quasi-experimental methods to construct treatment and control groups. The key idea here is exploiting randomness from external events that differentially expose subjects to a particular treatment. Here we review three quasi-experimental methods: difference-in-differences, instrumental variables and regression discontinuity (Fig. 3).
Difference-in-differences. Difference-in-difference regression (DiD) investigates the effect of an unexpected event, comparing the affected group (the treated group) with an unaffected group (the control group). The control group is intended to provide the counterfactual path—what would have happened were it not for the unexpected event. Ideally, the treated and control groups are on virtually identical paths before the treatment event, but DiD can also work if the groups are on parallel paths (Fig. 3a). For example, one study 226 examines how the premature death of superstar scientists affects the productivity of their previous collaborators. The control group are collaborators of superstars who did not die in the time frame. The two groups do not show significant differences in publications before a death event, yet upon the death of a star scientist, the treated collaborators on average experience a 5–8% decline in their quality-adjusted publication rates compared with the control group. DiD has wide applicability in the science of science, having been used to analyse the causal effects of grant design 24 , access costs to previous research 155,227 , university technology transfer policies 154 , intellectual property 228 , citation practices 229 , evolution of fields 221 and the impacts of paper retractions 230,231,232 . The DiD literature has grown especially rapidly in the field of economics, with substantial recent refinements 233,234 .
Instrumental variables. Another quasi-experimental approach utilizes ‘instrumental variables’ (IV). The goal is to determine the causal influence of some feature X on some outcome Y by using a third, instrumental variable. This instrumental variable is a quasi-random event that induces variation in X and, except for its impact through X, has no other effect on the outcome Y (Fig. 3b). For example, consider a study of astronomy that seeks to understand how telescope time affects career advancement 235 . Here, one cannot simply look at the correlation between telescope time and career outcomes because many confounds (such as talent or grit) may influence both telescope time and career opportunities. Now consider the weather as an instrumental variable. Cloudy weather will, at random, reduce an astronomer’s observational time. Yet, the weather on particular nights is unlikely to correlate with a scientist’s innate qualities. The weather can then provide an instrumental variable to reveal a causal relationship between telescope time and career outcomes. Instrumental variables have been used to study local peer effects in research 151 , the impact of gender composition in scientific committees 236 , patents on future innovation 237 and taxes on inventor mobility 238 .
Regression discontinuity. In regression discontinuity, policies with an arbitrary threshold for receiving some benefit can be used to construct treatment and control groups (Fig. 3c). Take the funding paylines for grant proposals as an example. Proposals with scores increasingly close to the payline are increasingly similar in their both observable and unobservable characteristics, yet only those projects with scores above the payline receive the funding. For example, a study 110 examines the effect of winning an early-career grant on the probability of winning a later, mid-career grant. The probability has a discontinuous jump across the initial grant’s payline, providing the treatment and control groups needed to estimate the causal effect of receiving a grant. This example utilizes the ‘sharp’ regression discontinuity that assumes treatment status to be fully determined by the cut-off. If we assume treatment status is only partly determined by the cut-off, we can use ‘fuzzy’ regression discontinuity designs. Here the probability of receiving a grant is used to estimate the future outcome 11,110,239,240,241 .
Although quasi-experiments are powerful tools, they face their own limitations. First, these approaches identify causal effects within a specific context and often engage small numbers of observations. How representative the samples are for broader populations or contexts is typically left as an open question. Second, the validity of the causal design is typically not ironclad. Researchers usually conduct different robustness checks to verify whether observable confounders have significant differences between the treated and control groups, before treatment. However, unobservable features may still differ between treatment and control groups. The quality of instrumental variables and the specific claim that they have no effect on the outcome except through the variable of interest, is also difficult to assess. Ultimately, researchers must rely partly on judgement to tell whether appropriate conditions are met for causal inference.
This section emphasized popular econometric approaches to causal inference. Other empirical approaches, such as graphical causal modelling 242,243 , also represent an important stream of work on assessing causal relationships. Such approaches usually represent causation as a directed acyclic graph, with nodes as variables and arrows between them as suspected causal relationships. In the science of science, the directed acyclic graph approach has been applied to quantify the causal effect of journal impact factor 244 and gender or racial bias 245 on citations. Graphical causal modelling has also triggered discussions on strengths and weaknesses compared to the econometrics methods 246,247 .
Experiments
In contrast to quasi-experimental approaches, laboratory and field experiments conduct direct randomization in assigning treatment and control groups. These methods engage explicitly in the data generation process, manipulating interventions to observe counterfactuals. These experiments are crafted to study mechanisms of specific interest and, by designing the experiment and formally randomizing, can produce especially rigorous causal inference.
Laboratory experiments. Laboratory experiments build counterfactual worlds in well-controlled laboratory environments. Researchers randomly assign participants to the treatment or control group and then manipulate the laboratory conditions to observe different outcomes in the two groups. For example, consider laboratory experiments on team performance and gender composition 144,248 . The researchers randomly assign participants into groups to perform tasks such as solving puzzles or brainstorming. Teams with a higher proportion of women are found to perform better on average, offering evidence that gender diversity is causally linked to team performance. Laboratory experiments can allow researchers to test forces that are otherwise hard to observe, such as how competition influences creativity 249 . Laboratory experiments have also been used to evaluate how journal impact factors shape scientists’ perceptions of rewards 250 and gender bias in hiring 251 .
Laboratory experiments allow for precise control of settings and procedures to isolate causal effects of interest. However, participants may behave differently in synthetic environments than in real-world settings, raising questions about the generalizability and replicability of the results 252,253,254 . To assess causal effects in real-world settings, researcher use randomized controlled trials.
Randomized controlled trials. A randomized controlled trial (RCT), or field experiment, is a staple for causal inference across a wide range of disciplines. RCTs randomly assign participants into the treatment and control conditions 255 and can be used not only to assess mechanisms but also to test real-world interventions such as policy change. The science of science has witnessed growing use of RCTs. For instance, a field experiment 146 investigated whether lower search costs for collaborators increased collaboration in grant applications. The authors randomly allocated principal investigators to face-to-face sessions in a medical school, and then measured participants’ chance of writing a grant proposal together. RCTs have also offered rich causal insights on peer review 256,257,258,259,260 and gender bias in science 261,262,263 .
While powerful, RCTs are difficult to conduct in the science of science, mainly for two reasons. The first concerns potential risks in a policy intervention. For instance, while randomizing funding across individuals could generate crucial causal insights for funders, it may also inadvertently harm participants’ careers 264 . Second, key questions in the science of science often require a long-time horizon to trace outcomes, which makes RCTs costly. It also raises the difficulty of replicating findings. A relative advantage of the quasi-experimental methods discussed earlier is that one can identify causal effects over potentially long periods of time in the historical record. On the other hand, quasi-experiments must be found as opposed to designed, and they often are not available for many questions of interest. While the best approaches are context dependent, a growing community of researchers is building platforms to facilitate RCTs for the science of science, aiming to lower their costs and increase their scale. Performing RCTs in partnership with science institutions can also contribute to timely, policy-relevant research that may substantially improve science decision-making and investments.
Outlook
Research in the science of science has been empowered by the growth of high-scale data, new measurement approaches and an expanding range of empirical methods. These tools provide enormous capacity to test conceptual frameworks about science, discover factors impacting scientific productivity, predict key scientific outcomes and design policies that better facilitate future scientific progress. A careful appreciation of empirical techniques can help researchers to choose effective tools for questions of interest and propel the field. A better and broader understanding of these methodologies may also build bridges across diverse research communities, facilitating communication and collaboration, and better leveraging the value of diverse perspectives. The science of science is about turning scientific methods on the nature of science itself. The fruits of this work, with time, can guide researchers and research institutions to greater progress in discovery and understanding across the landscape of scientific inquiry.
References
- Bush, V. Science–the Endless Frontier: A Report to the President on a Program for Postwar Scientific Research (National Science Foundation, 1990).
- Mokyr, J. The Gifts of Athena (Princeton Univ. Press, 2011).
- Jones, B. F. in Rebuilding the Post-Pandemic Economy (eds Kearney, M. S. & Ganz, A.) 272–310 (Aspen Institute Press, 2021).
- Wang, D. & Barabási, A.-L. The Science of Science (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2021).
- Fortunato, S. et al. Science of science. Science359, eaao0185 (2018). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Azoulay, P. et al. Toward a more scientific science. Science361, 1194–1197 (2018). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Clauset, A., Larremore, D. B. & Sinatra, R. Data-driven predictions in the science of science. Science355, 477–480 (2017). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Zeng, A. et al. The science of science: from the perspective of complex systems. Phys. Rep.714, 1–73 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Lin, Z., Yin. Y., Liu, L. & Wang, D. SciSciNet: a large-scale open data lake for the science of science research. Sci. Data,https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-023-02198-9 (2023).
- Ahmadpoor, M. & Jones, B. F. The dual frontier: patented inventions and prior scientific advance. Science357, 583–587 (2017). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Azoulay, P., Graff Zivin, J. S., Li, D. & Sampat, B. N. Public R&D investments and private-sector patenting: evidence from NIH funding rules. Rev. Econ. Stud.86, 117–152 (2019). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Yin, Y., Dong, Y., Wang, K., Wang, D. & Jones, B. F. Public use and public funding of science. Nat. Hum. Behav.6, 1344–1350 (2022). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Merton, R. K. The Sociology of Science: Theoretical and Empirical Investigations (Univ. Chicago Press, 1973).
- Kuhn, T. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions (Princeton Univ. Press, 2021).
- Uzzi, B., Mukherjee, S., Stringer, M. & Jones, B. Atypical combinations and scientific impact. Science342, 468–472 (2013). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Zuckerman, H. Scientific Elite: Nobel Laureates in the United States (Transaction Publishers, 1977).
- Wu, L., Wang, D. & Evans, J. A. Large teams develop and small teams disrupt science and technology. Nature566, 378–382 (2019). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Wuchty, S., Jones, B. F. & Uzzi, B. The increasing dominance of teams in production of knowledge. Science316, 1036–1039 (2007). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Foster, J. G., Rzhetsky, A. & Evans, J. A. Tradition and innovation in scientists’ research strategies. Am. Sociol. Rev.80, 875–908 (2015). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Wang, D., Song, C. & Barabási, A.-L. Quantifying long-term scientific impact. Science342, 127–132 (2013). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Clauset, A., Arbesman, S. & Larremore, D. B. Systematic inequality and hierarchy in faculty hiring networks. Sci. Adv.1, e1400005 (2015). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Ma, A., Mondragón, R. J. & Latora, V. Anatomy of funded research in science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA112, 14760–14765 (2015). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Ma, Y. & Uzzi, B. Scientific prize network predicts who pushes the boundaries of science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA115, 12608–12615 (2018). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Azoulay, P., Graff Zivin, J. S. & Manso, G. Incentives and creativity: evidence from the academic life sciences. RAND J. Econ.42, 527–554 (2011). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Schor, S. & Karten, I. Statistical evaluation of medical journal manuscripts. JAMA195, 1123–1128 (1966). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Platt, J. R. Strong inference: certain systematic methods of scientific thinking may produce much more rapid progress than others. Science146, 347–353 (1964). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Ioannidis, J. P. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med.2, e124 (2005). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Simonton, D. K. Career landmarks in science: individual differences and interdisciplinary contrasts. Dev. Psychol.27, 119 (1991). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Way, S. F., Morgan, A. C., Clauset, A. & Larremore, D. B. The misleading narrative of the canonical faculty productivity trajectory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA114, E9216–E9223 (2017). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Sinatra, R., Wang, D., Deville, P., Song, C. & Barabási, A.-L. Quantifying the evolution of individual scientific impact. Science354, aaf5239 (2016). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Liu, L. et al. Hot streaks in artistic, cultural, and scientific careers. Nature559, 396–399 (2018). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Liu, L., Dehmamy, N., Chown, J., Giles, C. L. & Wang, D. Understanding the onset of hot streaks across artistic, cultural, and scientific careers. Nat. Commun.12, 5392 (2021). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Squazzoni, F. et al. Peer review and gender bias: a study on 145 scholarly journals. Sci. Adv.7, eabd0299 (2021). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Hofstra, B. et al. The diversity–innovation paradox in science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA117, 9284–9291 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Huang, J., Gates, A. J., Sinatra, R. & Barabási, A.-L. Historical comparison of gender inequality in scientific careers across countries and disciplines. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA117, 4609–4616 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Gläser, J. & Laudel, G. Governing science: how science policy shapes research content. Eur. J. Sociol.57, 117–168 (2016). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Stephan, P. E. How Economics Shapes Science (Harvard Univ. Press, 2012).
- Garfield, E. & Sher, I. H. New factors in the evaluation of scientific literature through citation indexing. Am. Doc.14, 195–201 (1963). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- de Solla Price, D. J. Networks of scientific papers. Science149, 510–515 (1965). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Etzkowitz, H., Kemelgor, C. & Uzzi, B. Athena Unbound: The Advancement of Women in Science and Technology (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2000).
- Simonton, D. K. Scientific Genius: A Psychology of Science (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1988).
- Khabsa, M. & Giles, C. L. The number of scholarly documents on the public web. PLoS ONE9, e93949 (2014). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Xia, F., Wang, W., Bekele, T. M. & Liu, H. Big scholarly data: a survey. IEEE Trans. Big Data3, 18–35 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Evans, J. A. & Foster, J. G. Metaknowledge. Science331, 721–725 (2011). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Milojević, S. Quantifying the cognitive extent of science. J. Informetr.9, 962–973 (2015). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Rzhetsky, A., Foster, J. G., Foster, I. T. & Evans, J. A. Choosing experiments to accelerate collective discovery. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA112, 14569–14574 (2015). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Poncela-Casasnovas, J., Gerlach, M., Aguirre, N. & Amaral, L. A. Large-scale analysis of micro-level citation patterns reveals nuanced selection criteria. Nat. Hum. Behav.3, 568–575 (2019). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hardwicke, T. E. et al. Data availability, reusability, and analytic reproducibility: evaluating the impact of a mandatory open data policy at the journal Cognition. R. Soc. Open Sci.5, 180448 (2018). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Nagaraj, A., Shears, E. & de Vaan, M. Improving data access democratizes and diversifies science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA117, 23490–23498 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Bravo, G., Grimaldo, F., López-Iñesta, E., Mehmani, B. & Squazzoni, F. The effect of publishing peer review reports on referee behavior in five scholarly journals. Nat. Commun.10, 322 (2019). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Tran, D. et al. An open review of open review: a critical analysis of the machine learning conference review process. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2010.05137 (2020).
- Tshitoyan, V. et al. Unsupervised word embeddings capture latent knowledge from materials science literature. Nature571, 95–98 (2019). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Yang, Y., Wu, Y. & Uzzi, B. Estimating the deep replicability of scientific findings using human and artificial intelligence. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA117, 10762–10768 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Mukherjee, S., Uzzi, B., Jones, B. & Stringer, M. A new method for identifying recombinations of existing knowledge associated with high‐impact innovation. J. Prod. Innov. Manage.33, 224–236 (2016). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Leahey, E., Beckman, C. M. & Stanko, T. L. Prominent but less productive: the impact of interdisciplinarity on scientists’ research. Adm. Sci. Q.62, 105–139 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Sauermann, H. & Haeussler, C. Authorship and contribution disclosures. Sci. Adv.3, e1700404 (2017). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Oliveira, D. F. M., Ma, Y., Woodruff, T. K. & Uzzi, B. Comparison of National Institutes of Health grant amounts to first-time male and female principal investigators. JAMA321, 898–900 (2019). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Yang, Y., Chawla, N. V. & Uzzi, B. A network’s gender composition and communication pattern predict women’s leadership success. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA116, 2033–2038 (2019). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Way, S. F., Larremore, D. B. & Clauset, A. Gender, productivity, and prestige in computer science faculty hiring networks. In Proc. 25th International Conference on World Wide Web 1169–1179. (ACM 2016)
- Malmgren, R. D., Ottino, J. M. & Amaral, L. A. N. The role of mentorship in protege performance. Nature465, 622–626 (2010). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Ma, Y., Mukherjee, S. & Uzzi, B. Mentorship and protégé success in STEM fields. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA117, 14077–14083 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Börner, K. et al. Skill discrepancies between research, education, and jobs reveal the critical need to supply soft skills for the data economy. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA115, 12630–12637 (2018). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Biasi, B. & Ma, S. The Education-Innovation Gap (National Bureau of Economic Research Working papers, 2020).
- Bornmann, L. Do altmetrics point to the broader impact of research? An overview of benefits and disadvantages of altmetrics. J. Informetr.8, 895–903 (2014). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Cleary, E. G., Beierlein, J. M., Khanuja, N. S., McNamee, L. M. & Ledley, F. D. Contribution of NIH funding to new drug approvals 2010–2016. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA115, 2329–2334 (2018). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Spector, J. M., Harrison, R. S. & Fishman, M. C. Fundamental science behind today’s important medicines. Sci. Transl. Med.10, eaaq1787 (2018). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Haunschild, R. & Bornmann, L. How many scientific papers are mentioned in policy-related documents? An empirical investigation using Web of Science and Altmetric data. Scientometrics110, 1209–1216 (2017). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Yin, Y., Gao, J., Jones, B. F. & Wang, D. Coevolution of policy and science during the pandemic. Science371, 128–130 (2021). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Sugimoto, C. R., Work, S., Larivière, V. & Haustein, S. Scholarly use of social media and altmetrics: a review of the literature. J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol.68, 2037–2062 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Dunham, I. Human genes: time to follow the roads less traveled? PLoS Biol.16, e3000034 (2018). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Kustatscher, G. et al. Understudied proteins: opportunities and challenges for functional proteomics. Nat. Methods19, 774–779 (2022). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Rosenthal, R. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol. Bull.86, 638 (1979). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Franco, A., Malhotra, N. & Simonovits, G. Publication bias in the social sciences: unlocking the file drawer. Science345, 1502–1505 (2014). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Vera-Baceta, M.-A., Thelwall, M. & Kousha, K. Web of Science and Scopus language coverage. Scientometrics121, 1803–1813 (2019). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Waltman, L. A review of the literature on citation impact indicators. J. Informetr.10, 365–391 (2016). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Garfield, E. & Merton, R. K. Citation Indexing: Its Theory and Application in Science, Technology, and Humanities (Wiley, 1979).
- Kelly, B., Papanikolaou, D., Seru, A. & Taddy, M. Measuring Technological Innovation Over the Long Run Report No. 0898-2937 (National Bureau of Economic Research, 2018).
- Kogan, L., Papanikolaou, D., Seru, A. & Stoffman, N. Technological innovation, resource allocation, and growth. Q. J. Econ.132, 665–712 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Hall, B. H., Jaffe, A. & Trajtenberg, M. Market value and patent citations. RAND J. Econ.36, 16–38 (2005). Google Scholar
- Yan, E. & Ding, Y. Applying centrality measures to impact analysis: a coauthorship network analysis. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol.60, 2107–2118 (2009). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Radicchi, F., Fortunato, S., Markines, B. & Vespignani, A. Diffusion of scientific credits and the ranking of scientists. Phys. Rev. E80, 056103 (2009). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Bollen, J., Rodriquez, M. A. & Van de Sompel, H. Journal status. Scientometrics69, 669–687 (2006). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Bergstrom, C. T., West, J. D. & Wiseman, M. A. The eigenfactor™ metrics. J. Neurosci.28, 11433–11434 (2008). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Cronin, B. & Sugimoto, C. R. Beyond Bibliometrics: Harnessing Multidimensional Indicators of Scholarly Impact (MIT Press, 2014).
- Hicks, D., Wouters, P., Waltman, L., De Rijcke, S. & Rafols, I. Bibliometrics: the Leiden Manifesto for research metrics. Nature520, 429–431 (2015). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Catalini, C., Lacetera, N. & Oettl, A. The incidence and role of negative citations in science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA112, 13823–13826 (2015). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Alcacer, J. & Gittelman, M. Patent citations as a measure of knowledge flows: the influence of examiner citations. Rev. Econ. Stat.88, 774–779 (2006). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Ding, Y. et al. Content‐based citation analysis: the next generation of citation analysis. J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol.65, 1820–1833 (2014). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Teufel, S., Siddharthan, A. & Tidhar, D. Automatic classification of citation function. In Proc. 2006 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, 103–110 (Association for Computational Linguistics 2006)
- Seeber, M., Cattaneo, M., Meoli, M. & Malighetti, P. Self-citations as strategic response to the use of metrics for career decisions. Res. Policy48, 478–491 (2019). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Pendlebury, D. A. The use and misuse of journal metrics and other citation indicators. Arch. Immunol. Ther. Exp.57, 1–11 (2009). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Biagioli, M. Watch out for cheats in citation game. Nature535, 201 (2016). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Jo, W. S., Liu, L. & Wang, D. See further upon the giants: quantifying intellectual lineage in science. Quant. Sci. Stud.3, 319–330 (2022). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Boyack, K. W., Klavans, R. & Börner, K. Mapping the backbone of science. Scientometrics64, 351–374 (2005). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Gates, A. J., Ke, Q., Varol, O. & Barabási, A.-L. Nature’s reach: narrow work has broad impact. Nature575, 32–34 (2019). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Börner, K., Penumarthy, S., Meiss, M. & Ke, W. Mapping the diffusion of scholarly knowledge among major US research institutions. Scientometrics68, 415–426 (2006). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- King, D. A. The scientific impact of nations. Nature430, 311–316 (2004). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Pan, R. K., Kaski, K. & Fortunato, S. World citation and collaboration networks: uncovering the role of geography in science. Sci. Rep.2, 902 (2012). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Jaffe, A. B., Trajtenberg, M. & Henderson, R. Geographic localization of knowledge spillovers as evidenced by patent citations. Q. J. Econ.108, 577–598 (1993). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Funk, R. J. & Owen-Smith, J. A dynamic network measure of technological change. Manage. Sci.63, 791–817 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Yegros-Yegros, A., Rafols, I. & D’este, P. Does interdisciplinary research lead to higher citation impact? The different effect of proximal and distal interdisciplinarity. PLoS ONE10, e0135095 (2015). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Larivière, V., Haustein, S. & Börner, K. Long-distance interdisciplinarity leads to higher scientific impact. PLoS ONE10, e0122565 (2015). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Fleming, L., Greene, H., Li, G., Marx, M. & Yao, D. Government-funded research increasingly fuels innovation. Science364, 1139–1141 (2019). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bowen, A. & Casadevall, A. Increasing disparities between resource inputs and outcomes, as measured by certain health deliverables, in biomedical research. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA112, 11335–11340 (2015). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Li, D., Azoulay, P. & Sampat, B. N. The applied value of public investments in biomedical research. Science356, 78–81 (2017). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lehman, H. C. Age and Achievement (Princeton Univ. Press, 2017).
- Simonton, D. K. Creative productivity: a predictive and explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks. Psychol. Rev.104, 66 (1997). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Duch, J. et al. The possible role of resource requirements and academic career-choice risk on gender differences in publication rate and impact. PLoS ONE7, e51332 (2012). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Wang, Y., Jones, B. F. & Wang, D. Early-career setback and future career impact. Nat. Commun.10, 4331 (2019). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Bol, T., de Vaan, M. & van de Rijt, A. The Matthew effect in science funding. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA115, 4887–4890 (2018). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Jones, B. F. Age and great invention. Rev. Econ. Stat.92, 1–14 (2010). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Newman, M. Networks (Oxford Univ. Press, 2018).
- Mazloumian, A., Eom, Y.-H., Helbing, D., Lozano, S. & Fortunato, S. How citation boosts promote scientific paradigm shifts and nobel prizes. PLoS ONE6, e18975 (2011). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Hirsch, J. E. An index to quantify an individual’s scientific research output. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA102, 16569–16572 (2005). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Alonso, S., Cabrerizo, F. J., Herrera-Viedma, E. & Herrera, F. h-index: a review focused in its variants, computation and standardization for different scientific fields. J. Informetr.3, 273–289 (2009). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Egghe, L. An improvement of the h-index: the g-index. ISSI Newsl.2, 8–9 (2006). Google Scholar
- Kaur, J., Radicchi, F. & Menczer, F. Universality of scholarly impact metrics. J. Informetr.7, 924–932 (2013). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Majeti, D. et al. Scholar plot: design and evaluation of an information interface for faculty research performance. Front. Res. Metr. Anal.4, 6 (2020). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Sidiropoulos, A., Katsaros, D. & Manolopoulos, Y. Generalized Hirsch h-index for disclosing latent facts in citation networks. Scientometrics72, 253–280 (2007). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Jones, B. F. & Weinberg, B. A. Age dynamics in scientific creativity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA108, 18910–18914 (2011). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Dennis, W. Age and productivity among scientists. Science123, 724–725 (1956). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Sanyal, D. K., Bhowmick, P. K. & Das, P. P. A review of author name disambiguation techniques for the PubMed bibliographic database. J. Inf. Sci.47, 227–254 (2021). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Haak, L. L., Fenner, M., Paglione, L., Pentz, E. & Ratner, H. ORCID: a system to uniquely identify researchers. Learn. Publ.25, 259–264 (2012). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Malmgren, R. D., Ottino, J. M. & Amaral, L. A. N. The role of mentorship in protégé performance. Nature465, 662–667 (2010). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Oettl, A. Reconceptualizing stars: scientist helpfulness and peer performance. Manage. Sci.58, 1122–1140 (2012). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Morgan, A. C. et al. The unequal impact of parenthood in academia. Sci. Adv.7, eabd1996 (2021). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Morgan, A. C. et al. Socioeconomic roots of academic faculty. Nat. Hum. Behav.6, 1625–1633 (2022). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- San Francisco Declaration on Research Assessment (DORA) (American Society for Cell Biology, 2012).
- Falk‐Krzesinski, H. J. et al. Advancing the science of team science. Clin. Transl. Sci.3, 263–266 (2010). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Cooke, N. J. et al. Enhancing the Effectiveness of Team Science (National Academies Press, 2015).
- Börner, K. et al. A multi-level systems perspective for the science of team science. Sci. Transl. Med.2, 49cm24 (2010). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Leahey, E. From sole investigator to team scientist: trends in the practice and study of research collaboration. Annu. Rev. Sociol.42, 81–100 (2016). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- AlShebli, B. K., Rahwan, T. & Woon, W. L. The preeminence of ethnic diversity in scientific collaboration. Nat. Commun.9, 5163 (2018). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Hsiehchen, D., Espinoza, M. & Hsieh, A. Multinational teams and diseconomies of scale in collaborative research. Sci. Adv.1, e1500211 (2015). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Koning, R., Samila, S. & Ferguson, J.-P. Who do we invent for? Patents by women focus more on women’s health, but few women get to invent. Science372, 1345–1348 (2021). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Barabâsi, A.-L. et al. Evolution of the social network of scientific collaborations. Physica A311, 590–614 (2002). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Newman, M. E. Scientific collaboration networks. I. Network construction and fundamental results. Phys. Rev. E64, 016131 (2001). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Newman, M. E. Scientific collaboration networks. II. Shortest paths, weighted networks, and centrality. Phys. Rev. E64, 016132 (2001). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Palla, G., Barabási, A.-L. & Vicsek, T. Quantifying social group evolution. Nature446, 664–667 (2007). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Ross, M. B. et al. Women are credited less in science than men. Nature608, 135–145 (2022). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Shen, H.-W. & Barabási, A.-L. Collective credit allocation in science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA111, 12325–12330 (2014). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Merton, R. K. Matthew effect in science. Science159, 56–63 (1968). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Ni, C., Smith, E., Yuan, H., Larivière, V. & Sugimoto, C. R. The gendered nature of authorship. Sci. Adv.7, eabe4639 (2021). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Woolley, A. W., Chabris, C. F., Pentland, A., Hashmi, N. & Malone, T. W. Evidence for a collective intelligence factor in the performance of human groups. Science330, 686–688 (2010). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Feldon, D. F. et al. Postdocs’ lab engagement predicts trajectories of PhD students’ skill development. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA116, 20910–20916 (2019). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Boudreau, K. J. et al. A field experiment on search costs and the formation of scientific collaborations. Rev. Econ. Stat.99, 565–576 (2017). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Holcombe, A. O. Contributorship, not authorship: use CRediT to indicate who did what. Publications7, 48 (2019). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Murray, D. et al. Unsupervised embedding of trajectories captures the latent structure of mobility. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2012.02785 (2020).
- Deville, P. et al. Career on the move: geography, stratification, and scientific impact. Sci. Rep.4, 4770 (2014). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Edmunds, L. D. et al. Why do women choose or reject careers in academic medicine? A narrative review of empirical evidence. Lancet388, 2948–2958 (2016). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Waldinger, F. Peer effects in science: evidence from the dismissal of scientists in Nazi Germany. Rev. Econ. Stud.79, 838–861 (2012). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Agrawal, A., McHale, J. & Oettl, A. How stars matter: recruiting and peer effects in evolutionary biology. Res. Policy46, 853–867 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Fiore, S. M. Interdisciplinarity as teamwork: how the science of teams can inform team science. Small Group Res.39, 251–277 (2008). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Hvide, H. K. & Jones, B. F. University innovation and the professor’s privilege. Am. Econ. Rev.108, 1860–1898 (2018). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Murray, F., Aghion, P., Dewatripont, M., Kolev, J. & Stern, S. Of mice and academics: examining the effect of openness on innovation. Am. Econ. J. Econ. Policy8, 212–252 (2016). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Radicchi, F., Fortunato, S. & Castellano, C. Universality of citation distributions: toward an objective measure of scientific impact. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA105, 17268–17272 (2008). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Waltman, L., van Eck, N. J. & van Raan, A. F. Universality of citation distributions revisited. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol.63, 72–77 (2012). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Barabási, A.-L. & Albert, R. Emergence of scaling in random networks. Science286, 509–512 (1999). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- de Solla Price, D. A general theory of bibliometric and other cumulative advantage processes. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci.27, 292–306 (1976). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Cole, S. Age and scientific performance. Am. J. Sociol.84, 958–977 (1979). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Ke, Q., Ferrara, E., Radicchi, F. & Flammini, A. Defining and identifying sleeping beauties in science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA112, 7426–7431 (2015). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Bornmann, L., de Moya Anegón, F. & Leydesdorff, L. Do scientific advancements lean on the shoulders of giants? A bibliometric investigation of the Ortega hypothesis. PLoS ONE5, e13327 (2010). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Mukherjee, S., Romero, D. M., Jones, B. & Uzzi, B. The nearly universal link between the age of past knowledge and tomorrow’s breakthroughs in science and technology: the hotspot. Sci. Adv.3, e1601315 (2017). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Packalen, M. & Bhattacharya, J. NIH funding and the pursuit of edge science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA117, 12011–12016 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Zeng, A., Fan, Y., Di, Z., Wang, Y. & Havlin, S. Fresh teams are associated with original and multidisciplinary research. Nat. Hum. Behav.5, 1314–1322 (2021). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Newman, M. E. The structure of scientific collaboration networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA98, 404–409 (2001). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Larivière, V., Ni, C., Gingras, Y., Cronin, B. & Sugimoto, C. R. Bibliometrics: global gender disparities in science. Nature504, 211–213 (2013). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- West, J. D., Jacquet, J., King, M. M., Correll, S. J. & Bergstrom, C. T. The role of gender in scholarly authorship. PLoS ONE8, e66212 (2013). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Gao, J., Yin, Y., Myers, K. R., Lakhani, K. R. & Wang, D. Potentially long-lasting effects of the pandemic on scientists. Nat. Commun.12, 6188 (2021). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Jones, B. F., Wuchty, S. & Uzzi, B. Multi-university research teams: shifting impact, geography, and stratification in science. Science322, 1259–1262 (2008). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Chu, J. S. & Evans, J. A. Slowed canonical progress in large fields of science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA118, e2021636118 (2021). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Wang, J., Veugelers, R. & Stephan, P. Bias against novelty in science: a cautionary tale for users of bibliometric indicators. Res. Policy46, 1416–1436 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Stringer, M. J., Sales-Pardo, M. & Amaral, L. A. Statistical validation of a global model for the distribution of the ultimate number of citations accrued by papers published in a scientific journal. J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol.61, 1377–1385 (2010). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Bianconi, G. & Barabási, A.-L. Bose-Einstein condensation in complex networks. Phys. Rev. Lett.86, 5632 (2001). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bianconi, G. & Barabási, A.-L. Competition and multiscaling in evolving networks. Europhys. Lett.54, 436 (2001). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Yin, Y. & Wang, D. The time dimension of science: connecting the past to the future. J. Informetr.11, 608–621 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Pan, R. K., Petersen, A. M., Pammolli, F. & Fortunato, S. The memory of science: Inflation, myopia, and the knowledge network. J. Informetr.12, 656–678 (2018). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Yin, Y., Wang, Y., Evans, J. A. & Wang, D. Quantifying the dynamics of failure across science, startups and security. Nature575, 190–194 (2019). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Candia, C. & Uzzi, B. Quantifying the selective forgetting and integration of ideas in science and technology. Am. Psychol.76, 1067 (2021). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Milojević, S. Principles of scientific research team formation and evolution. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA111, 3984–3989 (2014). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Guimera, R., Uzzi, B., Spiro, J. & Amaral, L. A. N. Team assembly mechanisms determine collaboration network structure and team performance. Science308, 697–702 (2005). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Newman, M. E. Coauthorship networks and patterns of scientific collaboration. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA101, 5200–5205 (2004). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Newman, M. E. Clustering and preferential attachment in growing networks. Phys. Rev. E64, 025102 (2001). ArticleCASGoogle Scholar
- Iacopini, I., Milojević, S. & Latora, V. Network dynamics of innovation processes. Phys. Rev. Lett.120, 048301 (2018). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kuhn, T., Perc, M. & Helbing, D. Inheritance patterns in citation networks reveal scientific memes. Phys. Rev.4, 041036 (2014). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Jia, T., Wang, D. & Szymanski, B. K. Quantifying patterns of research-interest evolution. Nat. Hum. Behav.1, 0078 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Zeng, A. et al. Increasing trend of scientists to switch between topics. Nat. Commun.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-11401-8 (2019).
- Siudem, G., Żogała-Siudem, B., Cena, A. & Gagolewski, M. Three dimensions of scientific impact. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA117, 13896–13900 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Petersen, A. M. et al. Reputation and impact in academic careers. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA111, 15316–15321 (2014). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Jin, C., Song, C., Bjelland, J., Canright, G. & Wang, D. Emergence of scaling in complex substitutive systems. Nat. Hum. Behav.3, 837–846 (2019). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hofman, J. M. et al. Integrating explanation and prediction in computational social science. Nature595, 181–188 (2021). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lazer, D. et al. Computational social science. Science323, 721–723 (2009). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Lazer, D. M. et al. Computational social science: obstacles and opportunities. Science369, 1060–1062 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Albert, R. & Barabási, A.-L. Statistical mechanics of complex networks. Rev. Mod. Phys.74, 47 (2002). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Newman, M. E. The structure and function of complex networks. SIAM Rev.45, 167–256 (2003). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Song, C., Qu, Z., Blumm, N. & Barabási, A.-L. Limits of predictability in human mobility. Science327, 1018–1021 (2010). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Alessandretti, L., Aslak, U. & Lehmann, S. The scales of human mobility. Nature587, 402–407 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Pastor-Satorras, R. & Vespignani, A. Epidemic spreading in scale-free networks. Phys. Rev. Lett.86, 3200 (2001). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Pastor-Satorras, R., Castellano, C., Van Mieghem, P. & Vespignani, A. Epidemic processes in complex networks. Rev. Mod. Phys.87, 925 (2015). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Goodfellow, I., Bengio, Y. & Courville, A. Deep Learning (MIT Press, 2016).
- Bishop, C. M. Pattern Recognition and Machine Learning (Springer, 2006).
- Dong, Y., Johnson, R. A. & Chawla, N. V. Will this paper increase your h-index? Scientific impact prediction. In Proc. 8th ACM International Conference on Web Search and Data Mining, 149–158 (ACM 2015)
- Xiao, S. et al. On modeling and predicting individual paper citation count over time. In IJCAI, 2676–2682 (IJCAI, 2016)
- Fortunato, S. Community detection in graphs. Phys. Rep.486, 75–174 (2010). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Chen, C. Science mapping: a systematic review of the literature. J. Data Inf. Sci.2, 1–40 (2017). CASGoogle Scholar
- Van Eck, N. J. & Waltman, L. Citation-based clustering of publications using CitNetExplorer and VOSviewer. Scientometrics111, 1053–1070 (2017). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- LeCun, Y., Bengio, Y. & Hinton, G. Deep learning. Nature521, 436–444 (2015). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Senior, A. W. et al. Improved protein structure prediction using potentials from deep learning. Nature577, 706–710 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Krenn, M. & Zeilinger, A. Predicting research trends with semantic and neural networks with an application in quantum physics. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA117, 1910–1916 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Iten, R., Metger, T., Wilming, H., Del Rio, L. & Renner, R. Discovering physical concepts with neural networks. Phys. Rev. Lett.124, 010508 (2020). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Guimerà, R. et al. A Bayesian machine scientist to aid in the solution of challenging scientific problems. Sci. Adv.6, eaav6971 (2020). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Segler, M. H., Preuss, M. & Waller, M. P. Planning chemical syntheses with deep neural networks and symbolic AI. Nature555, 604–610 (2018). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Ryu, J. Y., Kim, H. U. & Lee, S. Y. Deep learning improves prediction of drug–drug and drug–food interactions. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA115, E4304–E4311 (2018). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Kermany, D. S. et al. Identifying medical diagnoses and treatable diseases by image-based deep learning. Cell172, 1122–1131.e9 (2018). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Peng, H., Ke, Q., Budak, C., Romero, D. M. & Ahn, Y.-Y. Neural embeddings of scholarly periodicals reveal complex disciplinary organizations. Sci. Adv.7, eabb9004 (2021). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Youyou, W., Yang, Y. & Uzzi, B. A discipline-wide investigation of the replicability of psychology papers over the past two decades. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA120, e2208863120 (2023). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Mehrabi, N., Morstatter, F., Saxena, N., Lerman, K. & Galstyan, A. A survey on bias and fairness in machine learning. ACM Computing Surveys (CSUR)54, 1–35 (2021). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Way, S. F., Morgan, A. C., Larremore, D. B. & Clauset, A. Productivity, prominence, and the effects of academic environment. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA116, 10729–10733 (2019). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Li, W., Aste, T., Caccioli, F. & Livan, G. Early coauthorship with top scientists predicts success in academic careers. Nat. Commun.10, 5170 (2019). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Hendry, D. F., Pagan, A. R. & Sargan, J. D. Dynamic specification. Handb. Econ.2, 1023–1100 (1984). Google Scholar
- Jin, C., Ma, Y. & Uzzi, B. Scientific prizes and the extraordinary growth of scientific topics. Nat. Commun.12, 5619 (2021). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Azoulay, P., Ganguli, I. & Zivin, J. G. The mobility of elite life scientists: professional and personal determinants. Res. Policy46, 573–590 (2017). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Slavova, K., Fosfuri, A. & De Castro, J. O. Learning by hiring: the effects of scientists’ inbound mobility on research performance in academia. Organ. Sci.27, 72–89 (2016). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Sarsons, H. Recognition for group work: gender differences in academia. Am. Econ. Rev.107, 141–145 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Campbell, L. G., Mehtani, S., Dozier, M. E. & Rinehart, J. Gender-heterogeneous working groups produce higher quality science. PLoS ONE8, e79147 (2013). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Azoulay, P., Graff Zivin, J. S. & Wang, J. Superstar extinction. Q. J. Econ.125, 549–589 (2010). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Furman, J. L. & Stern, S. Climbing atop the shoulders of giants: the impact of institutions on cumulative research. Am. Econ. Rev.101, 1933–1963 (2011). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Williams, H. L. Intellectual property rights and innovation: evidence from the human genome. J. Polit. Econ.121, 1–27 (2013). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Rubin, A. & Rubin, E. Systematic Bias in the Progress of Research. J. Polit. Econ.129, 2666–2719 (2021). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Lu, S. F., Jin, G. Z., Uzzi, B. & Jones, B. The retraction penalty: evidence from the Web of Science. Sci. Rep.3, 3146 (2013). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Jin, G. Z., Jones, B., Lu, S. F. & Uzzi, B. The reverse Matthew effect: consequences of retraction in scientific teams. Rev. Econ. Stat.101, 492–506 (2019). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Azoulay, P., Bonatti, A. & Krieger, J. L. The career effects of scandal: evidence from scientific retractions. Res. Policy46, 1552–1569 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Goodman-Bacon, A. Difference-in-differences with variation in treatment timing. J. Econ.225, 254–277 (2021). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Callaway, B. & Sant’Anna, P. H. Difference-in-differences with multiple time periods. J. Econ.225, 200–230 (2021). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Hill, R. Searching for Superstars: Research Risk and Talent Discovery in Astronomy Working Paper (Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2019).
- Bagues, M., Sylos-Labini, M. & Zinovyeva, N. Does the gender composition of scientific committees matter? Am. Econ. Rev.107, 1207–1238 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Sampat, B. & Williams, H. L. How do patents affect follow-on innovation? Evidence from the human genome. Am. Econ. Rev.109, 203–236 (2019). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Moretti, E. & Wilson, D. J. The effect of state taxes on the geographical location of top earners: evidence from star scientists. Am. Econ. Rev.107, 1858–1903 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Jacob, B. A. & Lefgren, L. The impact of research grant funding on scientific productivity. J. Public Econ.95, 1168–1177 (2011). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Li, D. Expertise versus bias in evaluation: evidence from the NIH. Am. Econ. J. Appl. Econ.9, 60–92 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Wang, Y., Jones, B. F. & Wang, D. Early-career setback and future career impact. Nat. Commun.10, 4331 (2019). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Pearl, J. Causal diagrams for empirical research. Biometrika82, 669–688 (1995). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Pearl, J. & Mackenzie, D. The Book of Why: The New Science of Cause and Effect (Basic Books, 2018).
- Traag, V. A. Inferring the causal effect of journals on citations. Quant. Sci. Stud.2, 496–504 (2021). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Traag, V. & Waltman, L. Causal foundations of bias, disparity and fairness. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2207.13665 (2022).
- Imbens, G. W. Potential outcome and directed acyclic graph approaches to causality: relevance for empirical practice in economics. J. Econ. Lit.58, 1129–1179 (2020). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Heckman, J. J. & Pinto, R. Causality and Econometrics (National Bureau of Economic Research, 2022).
- Aggarwal, I., Woolley, A. W., Chabris, C. F. & Malone, T. W. The impact of cognitive style diversity on implicit learning in teams. Front. Psychol.10, 112 (2019). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Balietti, S., Goldstone, R. L. & Helbing, D. Peer review and competition in the Art Exhibition Game. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA113, 8414–8419 (2016). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Paulus, F. M., Rademacher, L., Schäfer, T. A. J., Müller-Pinzler, L. & Krach, S. Journal impact factor shapes scientists’ reward signal in the prospect of publication. PLoS ONE10, e0142537 (2015). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Williams, W. M. & Ceci, S. J. National hiring experiments reveal 2:1 faculty preference for women on STEM tenure track. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA112, 5360–5365 (2015). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Collaboration, O. S. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science349, aac4716 (2015). ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Camerer, C. F. et al. Evaluating replicability of laboratory experiments in economics. Science351, 1433–1436 (2016). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Camerer, C. F. et al. Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015. Nat. Hum. Behav.2, 637–644 (2018). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Duflo, E. & Banerjee, A. Handbook of Field Experiments (Elsevier, 2017).
- Tomkins, A., Zhang, M. & Heavlin, W. D. Reviewer bias in single versus double-blind peer review. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA114, 12708–12713 (2017). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Blank, R. M. The effects of double-blind versus single-blind reviewing: experimental evidence from the American Economic Review. Am. Econ. Rev.81, 1041–1067 (1991). Google Scholar
- Boudreau, K. J., Guinan, E. C., Lakhani, K. R. & Riedl, C. Looking across and looking beyond the knowledge frontier: intellectual distance, novelty, and resource allocation in science. Manage. Sci.62, 2765–2783 (2016). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Lane, J. et al. When Do Experts Listen to Other Experts? The Role of Negative Information in Expert Evaluations for Novel Projects Working Paper #21-007 (Harvard Business School, 2020).
- Teplitskiy, M. et al. Do Experts Listen to Other Experts? Field Experimental Evidence from Scientific Peer Review (Harvard Business School, 2019).
- Moss-Racusin, C. A., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Graham, M. J. & Handelsman, J. Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA109, 16474–16479 (2012). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Forscher, P. S., Cox, W. T., Brauer, M. & Devine, P. G. Little race or gender bias in an experiment of initial review of NIH R01 grant proposals. Nat. Hum. Behav.3, 257–264 (2019). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Dennehy, T. C. & Dasgupta, N. Female peer mentors early in college increase women’s positive academic experiences and retention in engineering. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA114, 5964–5969 (2017). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Azoulay, P. Turn the scientific method on ourselves. Nature484, 31–32 (2012). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
Acknowledgements
The authors thank all members of the Center for Science of Science and Innovation (CSSI) for invaluable comments. This work was supported by the Air Force Office of Scientific Research under award number FA9550-19-1-0354, National Science Foundation grant SBE 1829344, and the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation G-2019-12485.